SpeechPathology.com Phone: 800-242-5183


EDU Healthcare Opportunities

Pearson's EBP Briefs: Wh-Question Intervention for Children With Language Disorders

Pearson's EBP Briefs: Wh-Question Intervention for Children With Language Disorders
Emily Diehm, PhD, CCC-SLP, Tala Abou-Dahech, MA
November 1, 2019
Share:

Editor's Note: The content of this article was published by Pearson Assessments in the journal, EBP Briefs.  It is offered for CEUs through SpeechPathology.com in cooperation with Pearson.  Click here for supplemental handouts. 

Learning Objectives

After this course, readers will be able to: 

  1. Identify the clinical question for an example case scenario
  2. List relevant search criteria for retrieving evidence related to the clinical question
  3. Identify factors to consider when evaluating the evidence
  4. Describe a clinical decision based on the evidence analysis

Structured Abstract

Clinical Question: For a school-age child with a language disorder, what therapy strategies are effective to improve the child’s Wh-question answering abilities?

Method: Systematic search and review

Study Sources: Academic Search Complete, ASHAWire, Education Full Text, Education Research Complete, ERIC, Google Scholar, Psychology & Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsycINFO

Search Terms: wh-questions OR wh questions AND school-age OR child* OR preschool AND intervention OR therapy OR treatment

Number of Included Studies: 4

Primary Results:

Speech-language pathologists should be aware of the developmental progression of answering Wh-questions, as well as what factors, beyond question type, that may influence children’s Wh-question performance. Children who struggle to answer Wh-questions may benefit from a variety of instructional techniques, including strategies that:

  1. increase the visual representation of complex syntactic movement (e.g., assign colors and shapes to parts of speech, phrases, or clauses);
  2. increase intrinsic motivation (e.g., immediately give the child an object associated with the question he/she answered);
  3. increase naturalness (e.g., use a typically developing peer to answer the clinician’s questions when the child is unable to do so); and
  4. increase the level of modeling, feedback, and support (e.g., use an “I do, we do, you do” structure to therapy sessions) for incorrect answers to questions. 

Conclusions: Few studies have analyzed the effectiveness of interventions to improve Wh-question answering abilities in children with language impairment. Of the studies that met our inclusion criteria, the majority included special populations (i.e., autism spectrum disorder, hearing loss). Only one study investigated intervention for children with developmental language disorder (DLD). These studies also covered a wide age range (3–17 years) and question types, and no two intervention studies investigated intervention for the same Wh-questions. Because children with DLD frequently experience difficulty answering Wh-questions, there is a critical need for more research on effective interventions.

Clinical Scenario

Briana is a graduate student in her third semester of a speech-language pathology program. She just received her clinical assignment for the summer semester—TJ, a 7-year-old boy, diagnosed with a developmental language disorder (DLD; often referred to as specific language impairment), who has come to her university’s on-campus speech, language, and hearing clinic for five years. As Briana flipped through TJ’s many years of norm-referenced assessment score forms and associated initial and final treatment plans, she noticed he had goals for answering Wh-questions across multiple semesters, with little evidence that the interventions were successful at improving his ability to answer a variety of question types. 

After discussing TJ’s history and the typical intervention activities used with two of his former graduate clinicians, Briana learned that both former clinicians provided therapy that asked TJ Wh-questions in a drill format (e.g., “Where are they going?” “Who is he going to play with?”) while engaging him in play-based activities or reading a book. Later, Briana searched through therapy ideas and activities on Teachers Pay Teachers and Pinterest and found multiple products that also seemed to test Wh-question knowledge rather than materials that encouraged the clinician to explicitly teach the differences among question responses. Briana knew the importance of modeling a target behavior before expecting the client to perform the behavior (e.g., “I do, we do, you do” approach; Pearson & Gallagher, 1983), yet felt that TJ’s last two semesters of intervention tested his question-answering abilities, seemingly without teaching him specific knowledge and skills to answer the questions correctly. In other words, Briana wondered if a lack of explicit instruction may be related to the client’s sustained poor performance. Before TJ came to the clinic next week for his session, Briana decided that she needed to learn more about best practices for Wh-question intervention because she did not want TJ to endure another semester of limited progress.

Background Information

Wh-Question Development

The ability to ask and answer Wh-questions is refined over many early developmental years. Children typically learn to answer what and where questions before why and when questions (e.g., Cairns & Hsu, 1978; Toler & Bankson, 1976). Parnell, Patterson, and Harding (1984) found a similar sequence of development, albeit more detailed, while investigating the question-answering abilities of 40 typically developing children between the ages of 3 and 6. Each participant was presented with a total 81 stimulus items, in reference to three levels of immediacy/availability (i.e., three-dimensional item/ object/action in the environment, two-dimensional picture stimulus, or hypothetical/routine event) across nine Wh-question forms (i.e., what + be, what + do, where + be, which + be, who + be, whose + be, why, when, and what happened). Parnell and colleagues observed that as children age, their ability to respond with accurate information (i.e., content) in a grammatically correct format (i.e., syntax) improved, especially when the question referenced people, items, or actions within the contextualized setting (i.e., the child could see, feel, hear, etc., the response to the question). The authors stressed the importance of a multifactorial analysis of children’s responses to Wh-questions (e.g., grammatically correct? accurate information?). In terms of producing grammatically correct responses to Wh-questions, the following sequence was observed, ranging from easiest to most difficult question type: where, which, what + be, who, what + do, when, whose, why, what happened. Children produced accurate responses, albeit with grammatical errors, according to the sequence: what + be, which, where, who, whose, what + do, why, when, what happened. With respect to asking questions, children also progress through a similar acquisition sequence for asking Wh-questions, beginning with mastery of asking what, where, and who questions, and subsequently, acquiring the ability to ask how, why, and when (Bloom, Merkin & Wooten, 1982). Overall, before a child’s fourth birthday, the majority of question forms should be mastered and allow a child to ask, as well as answer, questions. 

Wh-Question Abilities in Children With Language Disorders

Children with DLD (Deevy & Leonard, 2004; van der Lely, 1998) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Cadette, Wilson, Brady, Dukes, & Bennett, 2016; Vicker, 2006) often struggle to answer Wh-questions. The differences observed among children with impaired language skills and those for whom language has developed typically may be, in part, related to the finding that implicit exposure to Wh-question forms, although an effective language learning input for typically developing children (Valian & Casey, 2003), does not appear to facilitate development in the same manner for those children with language impairment (Bishop, Adams, & Rosen, 2006). 

Depending on the nature of the child’s language impairment, preschool- and school-age children with delayed language skills will likely progress through the developmental acquisition of Wh-question answering abilities similar to that of typically developing students, albeit at a slower rate (Goodwin, Fein, & Naigles, 2012), with comprehension of who, what-do, and where questions appearing sooner than comprehension of why, when, and how questions (Friemoth Lee & Ashemore, 1983; Parnell, Amerman, & Harting, 1986). Beyond the type of Wh-question asked to a child with language impairment, difficulty answering the question may also be related to other factors. Several studies have reported on the influence of Wh-phrase movement (e.g., object questions [What are you baking?] versus subject questions [Who is baking the cake?]), because the syntax of a question may require the person to rearrange the initial order of the arguments (see Friedmann & Norvogrodksy, 2011, for a review). Indeed, for children with DLD who present with specific impairments in syntax, subject Wh-questions are easier than object Wh-questions because the former question type requires “less syntactic rearranging” than the latter (Deevy & Leonard, 2004; Friedmann & Novogrodsky, 2011). Increased linguistic processing demands required to understand the question, which occurs when including more words per question, may also impact Wh-question performance because children with language impairment perform better on Wh-questions with fewer words (Deevy & Leonard, 2004). 

Clinical and Academic Relevance of Wh-Question

Wh-questions not only serve multiple functions in social interactions but also hold clinical and academic merit. Norm-referenced assessments and state academic standards commonly contain language related to question answering and question asking abilities. For example, norm-referenced assessments often measure an individual’s ability to answer or ask Wh-questions, sometimes with the use of a visual support (e.g., pictures, objects) about literal, hypothetical, or future events. Responses may be personal (i.e., in reference to the child), collective (i.e., in reference to a group, including the child), or related to “other(s)” (i.e., of no reference to the child). Furthermore, the Common Core State Standards (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010) addresses Wh-questions across several subcomponents of the English Language Arts Standards (e.g., Reading: Informational Text, Speaking & Listening) and grade levels; students are expected to ask and answer multiple question forms for a variety of reasons (e.g., to clear up confusion about the text, to gather additional information, to deepen understanding). 

Applying the Background Information to TJ

After completing research on the developmental progression of Wh-questions, as well as learning that children with language impairments struggle to develop developmentally appropriate abilities in this area, Briana felt confident that adding a goal related to answering Wh-questions was appropriate to include within TJ’s treatment plan, despite a history of limited growth. To determine which questions to address in therapy, Briana looked at the state academic standards. For TJ’s age and grade level, he is expected to “ask and answer such questions as who, what, where, when, why, and how to demonstrate an understanding of key details in a text” (National Governors Association for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010). Although Briana knew the importance of accurately measuring her client’s abilities at baseline, as well as aligning the client’s therapy goals to the knowledge and skills needed for mastery of state academic standards, she was less sure of how to provide high-quality, evidence-based intervention.

Clinical Question

Using the PICO format (Population, Intervention, Comparison/Control, Outcome) to frame her clinical question, Briana posed the following inquiry: For a school-age child with a language disorder (P), what therapy strategies (I) are effective to improve the child’s Wh-question answering abilities (O)? Because Briana was unaware of any specific treatment approaches for Wh-question intervention beyond simply asking the child questions, she did not include a comparison or control (C) within her question. 

Search for the Evidence

To locate as many intervention articles as possible, Briana thought it best to include broad search terms that addressed the nature of the intervention/outcome (Wh-questions OR Wh questions), contained a similar age range to her client (AND school-age OR child* OR preschool), and terms synonymous with intervention (AND intervention OR therapy OR treatment). Because Briana was still a university student and had access to the university’s library, she decided to search several databases including: the university library (ERIC, Academic Search Complete, Education Full Text, Education Research Complete, Psychology & Behavioral Sciences Collection, and PsycINFO); ASHAWire; and Google Scholar. Briana decided that she would only review the first 10 pages of results in Google Scholar (i.e., 100 articles) because sometimes the search engine returns irrelevant results.

Briana also decided to only analyze the results of experimental, quasi-experimental, or single-subject designs in which the researchers included participants between the ages of 3 and 21, as well as administered specific Wh-question progress monitoring probes and/or Wh-question pre- and posttest measures. In addition to these search terms and inclusion criteria, Briana limited her online search results to articles that were written (or could be translated) in English and were published in peer-reviewed journals between January 1980 and the time of the search (May 2018).

Evaluating the Evidence

Of the three sources Briana searched (i.e., university library databases, ASHAWire, Google Scholar), 240 articles were returned. Briana was overwhelmed at the idea of reading more than 200 articles but then realized that she could often make decisions whether or not to retain a search result by reading the title of the article. She looked for words that were familiar to her, such as intervention, therapy, and treatment, as well as research design terms, such as single-subject, randomized control trial, and experimental to determine if the article required further analysis. In situations where the title seemed a bit ambiguous, Briana read the abstract to determine if the study met her inclusion criteria (e.g., age, language, intervention for Wh-questions). The initial review of titles and abstracts provided Briana with 13 studies to review. Rather than reading all 13 articles in their entirety, Briana went directly to the Methods section for each article. After applying her inclusion criteria, Briana excluded 10 articles because of study design (n = 2; case study); a language other than English spoken by participants (n = 3; Afrikaans, Hebrew, Norwegian); and the absence of a dependent measure of answering Wh-questions (n = 5; interventions focused on asking, rather than answering questions) (see Figure 1). She also found an additional article that met her inclusion criteria from the reference list of one of the articles reviewed. Finally, Briana was left with four articles to read in their entirety to learn about the strategies to improve the Wh-question answering abilities in children with delayed language skills (see Table 1). 

Summaries of Included Studies

The four studies appeared to address multiple ages (i.e., preschool to high school) and question types (i.e., who, what, where, and which). Briana thought that rather than attempt to summarize the articles as a single approach to therapy, she would instead analyze the articles for intervention strategies and ideas that she could apply individually, or in conjunction with one another, into TJ’s therapy. 

The first study incorporated using a typically developing (TD) peer to provide a model and increase the naturalness of answering what + do questions. Within a sample of preschool-age children with hearing loss, Richels, Bobzien, Schwartz, Browning, and Hester (2016) employed a multiple-baseline design to investigate the role of teachers and peers as models of grammatical forms, including answering Wh-questions. During 6-minute intervention sessions, picture stimuli were presented simultaneously to a child with hearing loss and a TD peer. First, the clinician asked the peer what was happening in the picture. Then, after the peer responded, the child with hearing loss was asked the same question. Prompting procedures included verbal prompts (e.g., What did ___ say he was doing?”), verbal models (e.g., Yes, he is _____.”), recasting, acoustic highlighting, and praise. Overall, this study’s findings suggest that using a peer to model answering Wh-questions improved the question answering performance for the child with hearing loss, both immediately following intervention, as well as at a 6- to 10-week follow-up session.

The next study aimed to teach both asking and answering of where questions, again with preschool-age children. Koegel, Koegel, Green-Hopkins, and Barnes (2010) provided intervention to three children who were diagnosed with ASD. Intervention sessions lasted for 60 minutes and occurred two times per week over a span of approximately 4 to 10 weeks. Because the primary purpose of the intervention was to teach children how to ask “Where is the ____” in reference to an object hidden by the clinician, the clinician began the intervention by prompting the child with the verbal prompt, “Can you say, ‘Where is it?’” If the child repeated the question, the clinician then answered the question, including the targeted language structure (i.e., preposition or ordinal maker), so that the child would also be exposed to terms commonly used when answering where questions (e.g., under the basket, in front of the door). These targeted linguistic structures (i.e., prepositions and ordinal markers) were specifically chosen for each participant based on areas of need evidenced by pretest performance and were progress monitored every fifth intervention session. By the end of the intervention, all three participants produced the correct language structure in response to a where question. Although generalization data was collected for asking where questions (all participants asked where questions, unprompted, in 100% of the opportunities), there was no generalization data collected for answering where questions. The study’s findings indicated that a relatively high dose of individualized therapy (i.e., 60 minutes twice a week), with multiple models of specific linguistic structures, may be required for children with ASD. The treatment dose in this study was much higher than in the previous study for children with hearing loss (Richels et al., 2016), highlighting the need to consider the nature of a child’s language impairment when determining parameters of dose and frequency of treatment.

Perhaps the most explicit and complex of all of the reviewed interventions, was the study completed by Ebbels & van der Lely (2001) in which they provided intervention for four children, ages 11 to 13 years who were diagnosed with severe and persistent SLI, using a visual coding system to increase accurate comprehension and production of Wh-questions and passives. Initially, baseline measures of comprehension and production of passives and Wh-questions were assessed through using an “acting-out procedure” and a picture selection task from the Test of Active and Passive Sentences (TAPS; van der Lely, 1996). All participants received 13 half-hour sessions in which they were introduced to the visual coding system that consisted of color-coding parts of speech (e.g., yellow = verbs) and the use of shapes to represent phrasal and clausal structure (e.g., hexagon = verb phrase). Then, participants were split into two groups (2 participants per group), with one group receiving Wh-question therapy while the other group received instruction on passives, both for 10 weeks. Participants received the reverse therapy the following 10 weeks, along with an additional block of Wh-question therapy, because the interventionists determined that the skill required more instruction. Participants were taught to identify and use syntactic cues and received explicit instruction on grammatical rules (e.g., movement of tense). This study’s findings revealed that all participants improved production and comprehension of passives and Wh-questions, although questions that included syntactic movement (i.e., subject and object questions) negatively impacted some participants’ performance. 

More recently, Cadette et al. (2016) incorporated principles of direct instruction to teach three students with ASD (ages 15 to 17 years) how to answer who, what, and where questions. The authors selected these instructional targets because of participants’ performance on the WH Comprehension Test (Vicker, 2002) at baseline (i.e., What = 0–40%, Who = 0–20%, and Where = 0–20%). Intervention was embedded into the participants’ school curriculum, including modifications to include numerous opportunities for Wh-questions, teaching each question type in isolation, and only moving to the next question type after the participant achieved mastery on the previous. While participants looked at a picture, the interventionist 1) gave a verbal prompt, 2) referenced the picture/book, 3) asked a Wh-question, and 4) cued the students to respond using a hand signal. If a student did not answer the question correctly, the interventionist 1) provided a model of the correct answer (i.e., “I do”), 2) encouraged all students to say the correct answer along with her (i.e., “We do”), 3) asked the student to repeat the correct answer on his own (i.e., “You [all] do”), and then 4) repeated the initial question to ensure learning. At the completion of intervention (i.e., 20–30 instructional sessions in total), all students answered who and what questions with 100% accuracy and maintained mastery at the 2- and 4-week maintenance periods. The where question format appeared to be more difficult; one of the participants did not achieve mastery during the intervention period (average of 50% accuracy) and subsequently dropped to 25% accuracy at the 4-week maintenance probe. Although the other two participants reached mastery for answering where questions during the intervention, only one of these two students maintained that performance during the maintenance probes. Findings from this study indicated that systematic instruction—treating one Wh-question form at a time—paired with scaffolding strategies to promote independence, are effective strategies to promote mastery; however, ongoing “booster” sessions may be required to maintain these skills. 

Each of the four articles that Briana reviewed addressed a slightly different aspect pertaining to her original clinical question: For a school-age child with a language disorder, what therapy strategies are effective to improve the child’s Wh-question answering abilities? Because no two studies investigated the same Wh-question form, provided treatment to children of similar ages from the same population, or measured performance in the same way, Briana has several options for designing and delivering treatment for Wh-questions that are aligned with evidence from the literature. 

The Evidence-Based Decision

Because all of the studies in this review were single-subject designs, each of which employed multiple opportunities to observe the effect of intervention, Briana decided that she would not give more weight to any single study. Although none of the studies included participants who were the same age as her client, Briana decided that she could feasibly incorporate the evidence-based intervention strategies for TJ, because he still displayed difficulty answering some of the questions targeted within the interventions. Therefore, she developed the following set of evidence-based recommendations to include in her therapy for TJ: 

  1. Target Wh-questions in the same developmental order that typically developing children display, because there is no research to suggest that children with DLD comprehend Wh-questions in a different order (Cadette et al., 2016).
  2. Be aware of how syntactic structures (e.g., subject versus object Wh-question) and length of the question may impact the child’s performance (Ebbels, 2007; van der Lely, 1998).
  3. Consider asking questions that have answers visible in the immediate environment. Furthermore, allow the child to manipulate or hold an object, perform an action, etc., after verbally answering the question, because the use of items/people/actions in the immediate environment may serve as an intrinsic motivator and increase generalization (Koegel et al., 2010).
  4. When manipulatives may not be feasible for a question/answer (e.g., Why is she sad?), or perhaps in conjunction with using manipulatives, consider incorporating a typically developing peer into the session to serve as a language model and to increase the naturalness of asking questions and having someone else, other than you (the clinician), answer (Richels et al., 2016).
  5. For children whose goals target more complex Wh-questions (e.g., how, why, when), or for whom, perhaps, other methods have been unsuccessful, explicitly teach the parts of speech associated with each question type, as well as how syntactic movement may influence the correct answer, using visual symbols (e.g., color coding, shapes). Make sure to present the visual symbols in a systematic manner, only presenting new symbols when required (Ebbels & van der Lely, 2001). 
  6. Provide levels of support for incorrect responses, including a model of the correct answer. After giving the model (i.e., I do), encourage the student to repeat the answer with you (i.e., We do), followed by asking the student to say the answer independently (i.e., You do) (Cadette et al., 2016).

Although none of the studies included in Briana’s synthesis addressed her initial concern that the Wh-question intervention previously provided to TJ lacked explicit instruction in how to answer each question type (e.g., Who. If I hear the word who in a sentence, it means that my answer will usually include a person or animal.), Briana knew that the provision of evidence-based treatment extended beyond using only information discovered during a literature search and also included her professional knowledge/expertise and a consideration for the client’s perspective, such as the client’s circumstances, preferences, and areas of need (e.g., Dollaghan, 2007; Sackett, Straus, Richardson, Rosenberg, & Haynes, 2000). Therefore, Briana decided that in addition to incorporating the recommendations from the literature, she would rely on her professional knowledge and experience to develop a systematic treatment program that also provided TJ with explicit descriptions of how to answer each Wh-question targeted during therapy, with ongoing “booster” sessions provided as needed to ensure that TJ would be able to answer a variety of Wh-questions during functional and academic activities. With treatment beginning next week, Briana now feels confident in her approach to therapy this semester and is hopeful that incorporating evidence-based strategies will improve TJ’s performance on Wh-questions so that she will be able to make recommendations for new goals at the end of the semester, now that TJ has had the same goals in place for multiple semesters.

References

* indicates that the reference was included in the final systematic search and review.

Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., & Rosen, S. (2006). Resistance of grammatical impairment to computerized comprehension training in children with specific and non-specific language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(1), 19–40. doi:10.1080/13682820500144000

Bloom, L., Merkin, S., & Wootten, J. (1982). Wh-questions: Linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development, 53(4), 1084–1092. doi:10.2307/1129150

*Cadette, J. N., Wilson, C. L., Brady, M. P., Dukes, C., & Bennett, K. D. (2016). The effectiveness of direct instruction in teaching students with autism spectrum disorder to answer “Wh-” questions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(9), 2968–2978. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2825-2

Cairns, H. S., & Hsu, J. R. (1978). Who, why, when, and how: A developmental study. Journal of Child Language, 5(3), 478–488. doi:10.1017/S0305000900002105

Deevy, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2004). The comprehension of wh-questions with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(4), 802–815. doi:10.1044/1092- 4388(2004/060)

Dollaghan, C. A. (2007). The handbook for evidence-based practice in communication disorders. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

Ebbels, S. (2007). Teaching grammar to school-aged children with specific language impairment using shape coding. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 23(1), 67–93. doi:10.1191/0265659007072143

*Ebbels, S., & van der Lely, H. (2001). Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for children with severe persistent SLI. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(S1), 345–350. doi:10.3109/13682820109177909

Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2011). Which questions are most difficult to understand?: The comprehension of Wh questions in three subtypes of SLI. Lingua, 121(3), 367–382. doi:10.1016/j. lingua.2010.10.004

Friemoth Lee, R., & Ashmore, L. L. (1983). Receptive and expressive wh-question performance by language delayed children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 16(2), 99–109. doi:10.1016/0021-9924(83)90041-2

Goodwin, A., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2012). Comprehension of wh-questions precedes their production in typical development and autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 5(2), 109–123. doi:10.1002/aur.1220

*Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Green-Hopkins, I., & Barnes, C. C. (2010). Brief report: Question-asking and collateral language acquisition in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 509–515. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0896-z

National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common Core state standards for ELA/literacy: College and career readiness anchor standards for language: CCSS. ELA-LITERACY.RL.2.1. Retrieved from http://www. corestandards.org/ELA-Literacy/CCRA/L/#CCSS. ELALiteracy

Parnell, M. M., Amerman, J. D., & Harting, R. D. (1986). Responses of language-disordered children to wh-questions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17(2), 95–106. doi:10.1044/0161- 1461.1702.95

Parnell, M. M., Patterson, S. S., & Harding, M. A. (1984). Answers to wh-questions: A developmental study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 27(2), 297–305. doi:10.1044/jshr.2702.297

Pearson, P. D., & Gallagher, M. C. (1983). The instruction of reading comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8(3), 317–344. doi:10.1016/0361- 476X(83)90019-X

*Richels, C. G., Bobzien, J. L., Schwartz, K. S., Raver, S. A., Browning, E. L., & Hester, P. P. (2016). Teachers and peers as communication models to teach grammatical forms to preschoolers with hearing loss. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 37(3), 131–140. doi:10.1177/1525740115598770

Sackett, D. L., Straus, S., Richardson, W. S., Rosenberg, W., & Haynes, R. B. (2000). Evidence-based medicine: How to practice and teach EBM (2nd ed.). Edinburgh, Scotland: Churchill Livingstone.

Toler, S. A., & Bankson, N. W. (1976). Utilization of an interrogative model to evaluate mothers’ use and children’s comprehension of question forms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41(3), 301–314. doi:10.1044/jshd.4103.301

Valian, V., & Casey, L. (2003). Young children’s acquisition of wh-questions: The role of structured input. Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 117–143. doi:10.1017/ S0305000902005457

Van der Lely, H. (1998). SLI in children: Movement, economy, and deficits in the computations-syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7(2–4), 161–192. doi:10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_4

Van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996). Test of Active and Passive Sentences (TAPS) [Measurement instrument]. London, England: University College, Centre for Developmental Language Disorders and Cognitive Neuroscience.

Vicker, B. (2002). The WH question comprehension test: Exploring the world of WH question comprehension for students with an autism spectrum disorder. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Indiana Institute on Disability and Community.

Vicker, B. (2006). WH question comprehension and students with autism spectrum disorder. Perspectives on School-Based Issues, 7(1), 16–19. doi:10.1044/sbi7.1.16

Reprinted from: 

Abou-Dahech, T. and Diehm, E. A. (2019). Wh-question intervention for children with language disorders. EBP Briefs, 13(3), 1–9. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.


Emily Diehm, PhD, CCC-SLP

Dr. Diehm is a certified speech-language pathologist and assistant professor at the University of Toledo. Her clinical and research interests include how to best assess and provide intervention for children with oral language disorders, especially those children who are at-risk for later written-language disorders.  


Tala Abou-Dahech, MA

Tala Abou-Dahech, MA, CF-SLP, is a newly graduated speech-language pathologist from the University of Toledo. She is currently completing her clinical fellowship year serving adults at a skilled nursing and rehabilitation facility. Her research interests include psychosocial aspects of stuttering and language development. 



Related Courses

Facilitating First Verbs through Shared Book Reading
Presented by Susan Hendler Lederer, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9735Level: Introductory1 Hour
This course discusses early verb acquisition, choosing first verb targets, and a variety of strategies to facilitate verb learning using children’s picture books as a therapy context.

Dyslexia and Developmental Language Disorder: School-Based Screenings, Assessments, Interventions, and Implementation Science
Presented by Tiffany Hogan, PhD, CCC-SLP, FASHA
Video
Course: #11016Level: Intermediate1 Hour
Developmental language disorder (DLD) – a neurodiversity that occurs in 10% of the population and that makes learning vocabulary and grammar difficult - is the most commonly co-occurring learning disability associated with dyslexia, a word reading difficulty. DLD and dyslexia are described in this course with a focus on how they are the same and different. Actionable, practice solutions for improving educational outcomes for those with DLD and dyslexia through response to intervention and implementation science are discussed.

Language Outcomes of Children with Trauma Histories: Understanding the Impact
Presented by Yvette D. Hyter, PhD, CCC-SLP, ASHA Fellow
Video
Course: #9737Level: Advanced1.5 Hours
This is Part 1 of a two-part series. This course is designed to explain the influences that various types of childhood experiences with trauma and maltreatment have on development. The focus is on language and social-pragmatic communication skills of children.

Children with Trauma Histories: Assessment, Intervention, and Advocacy
Presented by Yvette D. Hyter, PhD, CCC-SLP, ASHA Fellow
Video
Course: #9738Level: Advanced1.5 Hours
This is Part 2 of a two-part series. This course features assessment protocols for determining language abilities of children with trauma histories and evidence-based, trauma-informed intervention strategies. It discusses the responsibility of trauma-informed speech, language and hearing professionals to advocate for children who have experienced trauma.

Reading Comprehension and the SLP: Foundational Understanding
Presented by Angie Neal, MS, CCC-SLP
Audio
Course: #10763Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This is Part 1 of a two-part series. This course provides SLPs with foundational knowledge needed to directly address and collaboratively support reading comprehension across all grade levels. Models of language and reading comprehension, comprehension processes vs. products, instruction in comprehension skills vs. strategies, factors in reading comprehension difficulties, and connections to general education are discussed.

Our site uses cookies to improve your experience. By using our site, you agree to our Privacy Policy.