SpeechPathology.com Phone: 800-242-5183


Aequor Allied - November 2024

The Dynamics of Learning to Hear New Speech Sounds

The Dynamics of Learning to Hear New Speech Sounds
Pamela Case, MA, CCC-SLP, Betty Tuller, J.A.S. Kelso
November 17, 2003
Share:

WHAT'S IRONY?In this paper I address the issue of difference or similarity in processing literal and nonliteral - ironic - uses. However, before dealing with this question, it is essential that we first reflect, even briefly, on what irony means.

Various theories have come up with different responses to this enigma (For brevity, I consider here only a fraction of contemporary views. The reader is advised to see Giora, 2003, Chapter 4). Following are three examples demonstrating ironic versus literal intention.

In example 1a and 1b, the same final statement is used, while the biasing context offers slight differences, different intentions are demonstrated, with conclusive identical wording, representing ironic and literal intention.

Example:

1a- Just how far have women risen in the film community? According to M. P., who was at the "Woman in Film" luncheon recently held in Los Angeles, it has actually been a very good year for women: Demi Moore was sold to Robert Redford for $1 million in the movie Indecent Proposal. Uma Thurman went for $40,000 to Robert De Niro in the recent movie, Mad Dog and Glory. ''Just three years ago, in Pretty Woman, Richard Gere bought Julia Roberts for - what was it? $3,000?
''I'd say women have had real progress.'' 1

1b- Just how far have women risen in the film community? According to M. P., who was at the "Woman in Film" luncheon recently held in Los Angeles, it has actually been a very good year for women: Demi Moore earned $10 million in the movie Indecent Proposal... Uma Thurman made $400,000 in the recent movie, Mad Dog and Glory. ''Just three years ago, in Pretty Woman, Julia Roberts earned - what was it? $130,000?
''I'd say women have had real progress.''

Most people will find (1a) ironic as opposed to (1b) which they would probably consider literal.

Below is another example. Most people would agree that while the first example (2a) is literally intended, the second (2b) invites an ironic reading:

Example:

2a. Tal and Ortal, the twins, wanted to go to the movies. Their mother recommended a movie she had seen shortly before. When they came home, she was eager to know how they found the movie. They both agreed: ''Very funny.''

2b. Iris was walking on her own in the dark alley, when all of a sudden a hand was laid on her back. Startled, she turned around to find out that the hand was her young brother's who sneaked behind her to frighten her. She said to him: ''Very funny''.2

And again, along the same line, most readers would take the first example (3a) to be metaphoric, whereas the second (3b) would be read as ironic:

Example:

3a. A friend was talking to Jodie about his niece and nephew who had visited him recently. He had really enjoyed having the children around. During the conversation the man said: Children are precious gems. This made Jodie think about her cousins.

3b.A friend was talking to Jodie about his niece and nephew who had visited him recently. He had really disliked having the children around. During the conversation the man said: Children are precious gems. This made Jodie think about her cousins.3

Would these identical utterances embedded in differently biasing contexts involve different comprehension mechanisms or would they follow similar processing routes?

The Standard Pragmatic Model (Grice, 1975)

According to The Standard Pragmatic Model, irony is the opposite of what is said.

Example 4: He says "It's a lovely day for a picnic." They go for a picnic and it rains. She says (sarcastically) "It's a lovely day for a picnic, indeed."

The implicature is that it's a lousy day for a picnic.

The Echoic Mention View (Relevance Theory, Sperber & Wilson, 1986/95)

The Echoic Mention View disputes The Standard Pragmatic Model. Instead of implicating the opposite, Sperber and Wilson argue that irony is used to get across a dissociative attitude from an opinion or thought echoed by the ironic statement: ''Verbal irony consists in echoing a tacitly attributed thought or utterance with a tacitly dissociative attitude'' (Wilson & Sperber, in press). Thus, in both the previous and the upcoming example ''there is an echoic allusion to be picked up. In the circumstances described, it is clear that the speaker of the following example (5) endorses the opinion echoed, whereas the speaker of the previous example rejects it with scorn. These utterances are interpreted on exactly similar patterns; the only difference is in the attitudes they express'' (Sperber & Wilson 1986/95: 239).

Example 5:
He: "It's a lovely day for a picnic." They go for a picnic and the sun shines.
She: (happily): "It's a lovely day for a picnic, indeed."

The Indirect Negation View (Giora, 1995)

According to the indirect negation view, the ironist's aim is to draw attention to a failed expectation - to some significant disparity between what is said, which would usually allude to some expectation - and what is. Whereas explicit negation only mitigates the negated concept (Giora, 1995; Giora, Balaban, Fein, & Alkabets, in press) irony, which is a form of indirect negation, highlights the gap between what is said and the situation criticized.

Note that, in Example 5 there is hardly any inconsistency between what is said and what is referred to. In contrast, in Example 4, which is ironic, there is. According to the indirect negation view, then, what Example 4 implicates, is that the day under discussion is far from being (what it should have been) - "lovely."

HOW IS IRONY UNDERSTOOD?

Apparently, there are two sources of knowledge that affect comprehension - linguistic knowledge and contextual information. The question, however, is which reigns supreme and when?

Initial processes

Do both linguistic knowledge and contextual information affect initial processes? To illustrate what might surface in initial processes, let's take a look at a contemporary Israeli irony (below):

Example 6:

The Israeli Chief of Staff: The Israeli army is indeed the most moral army in the world.

A radical leftist (mockingly): The Israeli army is indeed the most moral army in the world.

If it is contextual information - our knowledge of the critical attitude of Israeli radicals' to the military - that plays a primary role in initial processes of Example 6, then the ironic 'corrupt' interpretation of the utterance would be activated immediately, with no recourse to the conventional interpretation of moral. However, if it is the coded meaning of the individual words and expressions - e.g., the prominence of the 'ethics' sense of moral - that dominates initial comprehension, then both presentations in Example 6 would be processed literally initially, irrespective of contextual information.


Pamela Case, MA, CCC-SLP


Betty Tuller


J.A.S. Kelso



Related Courses

Treatment Approach Considerations for School-Aged Children with Speech Sound Disorders
Presented by Kathryn Cabbage, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9472Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This course will address the theoretical underpinnings and research base related to differential diagnosis and treatment of articulation and phonological deficits in children with speech sound disorders. Special considerations for how to tailor evaluation and intervention to meet the needs of school-age children will be discussed.

Back to Basics: Down Syndrome
Presented by Theresa Bartolotta, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #8975Level: Introductory1 Hour
This course serves as a primer on Down syndrome for practicing speech-language pathologists. The basics of the syndrome and common speech, language, voice, and fluency issues are addressed. Effective treatment strategies for improving communication across the lifespan are discussed.

20Q: Dynamics of School-Based Speech and Language Therapy Variables
Presented by Kelly Farquharson, PhD, CCC-SLP, Anne Reed, MS, CCC-SLP
Text
Course: #10002Level: Advanced1 Hour
This course reviews dynamics of speech and language therapy variables such as session frequency, intervention intensity, and dosage, and how these are impacted by different service delivery models. It discusses how therapy outcomes are related to therapy quality, IEP goals, and SLP-level variables such as job satisfaction and caseload size.

Sleuthing for /s/ and /r/: Facilitating Strategies for Residual Sound Errors
Presented by Lynn Berk, MA, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9237Level: Introductory2 Hours
This course will discuss the rationale and strategies for teaching production of /s/ and /r/ for upper elementary school-age children and older. Errors on these two sounds are considered residual when production continues to be inaccurate beyond the developmental age of acquisition.

20Q: A Continuum Approach for Sorting Out Processing Disorders
Presented by Gail J. Richard, PhD, CCC-SLP
Text
Course: #10008Level: Intermediate1 Hour
There is a good deal of confusion among audiologists and speech-language pathologists when a diagnosis of “processing disorder” is introduced. This course presents a continuum model to differentiate processing disorders into acoustic, phonemic, or linguistic aspects so that assessment and treatment can become more focused and effective. The roles of audiologists and SLPs in relation to processing disorders are described, and compensatory strategies for differing aspects of processing are presented.

Our site uses cookies to improve your experience. By using our site, you agree to our Privacy Policy.